Team:SUSTC-Shenzhen/WikiPlay/test
From 2014.igem.org
Line 16: | Line 16: | ||
[[Psychologist]]s such as [[John B. Watson]], [[Robert Plutchik]], and [[Paul Ekman]] have suggested that there is only a small set of basic or [[innate]] [[emotion]]s and that fear is one of them. This hypothesized set includes such emotions as [[happiness|joy]], [[sadness]], [[fright (fear)|fright]], [[dread]], [[Horror (emotion)|horror]], [[panic]], [[anxiety]], [[acute stress reaction]] and [[anger]]. | [[Psychologist]]s such as [[John B. Watson]], [[Robert Plutchik]], and [[Paul Ekman]] have suggested that there is only a small set of basic or [[innate]] [[emotion]]s and that fear is one of them. This hypothesized set includes such emotions as [[happiness|joy]], [[sadness]], [[fright (fear)|fright]], [[dread]], [[Horror (emotion)|horror]], [[panic]], [[anxiety]], [[acute stress reaction]] and [[anger]]. | ||
- | Fear should be distinguished from, but is closely related to, the emotion [[anxiety]], which occurs as the result of threats which are perceived to be uncontrollable or unavoidable. | + | Fear should be distinguished from, but is closely related to, the emotion [[anxiety]], which occurs as the result of threats which are perceived to be uncontrollable or unavoidable. |
- | The fear response serves survival by generating appropriate behavioral responses, as it has been preserved throughout [[evolution]]. | + | The fear response serves survival by generating appropriate behavioral responses, as it has been preserved throughout [[evolution]]. |
==Etymology== | ==Etymology== | ||
- | The noun '''fear''' stems from the Middle English feer, fere, fer, the Old English fǣr for 'calamity, danger' (and its verb fǣran 'frighten', but also 'revere') is related to the Proto-Germanic fērą 'danger', the Proto-Indo-European *per- 'to attempt, try, research, risk'. Fear is translated into German with Gefahr, into Dutch with gevaar, into Swedish with fara, into Albanian with frikë, and into Latin with perīculum, which is the root for the term in the [[Romanic language]]s. | + | The noun '''fear''' stems from the Middle English feer, fere, fer, the Old English fǣr for 'calamity, danger' (and its verb fǣran 'frighten', but also 'revere') is related to the Proto-Germanic fērą 'danger', the Proto-Indo-European *per- 'to attempt, try, research, risk'. Fear is translated into German with Gefahr, into Dutch with gevaar, into Swedish with fara, into Albanian with frikë, and into Latin with perīculum, which is the root for the term in the [[Romanic language]]s. |
- | The noun fear can be used in three ways with different meanings: In the uncountable form fear is a strong, uncontrollable, unpleasant emotion caused by actual or perceived danger, e.g. 'He was struck by fear on seeing the snake'. In the countable form, and when used with the indefinite article "a fear" means a phobia, a sense of fear induced by something or someone, e.g. 'Not everybody has the same fears; I have a fear of ants.' In an uncountable form it can also mean extreme veneration or awe, as toward a supreme being or deity. | + | The noun fear can be used in three ways with different meanings: In the uncountable form fear is a strong, uncontrollable, unpleasant emotion caused by actual or perceived danger, e.g. 'He was struck by fear on seeing the snake'. In the countable form, and when used with the indefinite article "a fear" means a phobia, a sense of fear induced by something or someone, e.g. 'Not everybody has the same fears; I have a fear of ants.' In an uncountable form it can also mean extreme veneration or awe, as toward a supreme being or deity. |
==Types== | ==Types== | ||
===Top 10 types of fear in the U.S.=== | ===Top 10 types of fear in the U.S.=== | ||
- | In a 2005 [[Gallup poll]] (U.S.A.), a national sample of adolescents between the ages of 13 and 17 were asked what they feared the most. The question was open-ended and participants were able to say whatever they wanted. The top ten fears were, in order: [[Terrorism|terrorist attacks]], [[spiders]], [[death]], [[failure|being a failure]], [[war]], [[Fear of crime|criminal]] or [[gang violence]], [[Solitude|being alone]], the [[future]], and [[nuclear war]]. | + | In a 2005 [[Gallup poll]] (U.S.A.), a national sample of adolescents between the ages of 13 and 17 were asked what they feared the most. The question was open-ended and participants were able to say whatever they wanted. The top ten fears were, in order: [[Terrorism|terrorist attacks]], [[spiders]], [[death]], [[failure|being a failure]], [[war]], [[Fear of crime|criminal]] or [[gang violence]], [[Solitude|being alone]], the [[future]], and [[nuclear war]]. |
- | In an estimate of what people fear the most, book author Bill Tancer analyzed the most frequent online queries that involved the phrase, "fear of..." following the assumption that people tend to seek information on the issues that concern them the most. His top ten list of fears published 2008 consisted of [[Aviation|flying]], [[Acrophobia|heights]], [[clown]]s, [[intimacy]], [[death]], [[Social rejection|rejection]], [[people]], [[snakes]], [[failure]], and [[driving]]. | + | In an estimate of what people fear the most, book author Bill Tancer analyzed the most frequent online queries that involved the phrase, "fear of..." following the assumption that people tend to seek information on the issues that concern them the most. His top ten list of fears published 2008 consisted of [[Aviation|flying]], [[Acrophobia|heights]], [[clown]]s, [[intimacy]], [[death]], [[Social rejection|rejection]], [[people]], [[snakes]], [[failure]], and [[driving]]. |
===Common phobias=== | ===Common phobias=== | ||
Line 40: | Line 40: | ||
===Fear of survival=== | ===Fear of survival=== | ||
- | According to Irfan Jamil, [[coadjutor bishop]] of [[Lahore]], as the world constantly changes, the greatest fear is the fear of survival. The social, economical, spiritual, political and educational circumstances in life make survival difficult in regard to such pressures that can come out of it. | + | According to Irfan Jamil, [[coadjutor bishop]] of [[Lahore]], as the world constantly changes, the greatest fear is the fear of survival. The social, economical, spiritual, political and educational circumstances in life make survival difficult in regard to such pressures that can come out of it. {{Better source|reason=citation not NPOV|date=February 2014}} |
===Fear of death=== | ===Fear of death=== | ||
- | Death anxiety is multidimensional; it covers "fears related to one's own death, the death of others, fear of the unknown after death, fear of obliteration, and fear of the dying process, which includes fear of a slow death and a painful death". | + | Death anxiety is multidimensional; it covers "fears related to one's own death, the death of others, fear of the unknown after death, fear of obliteration, and fear of the dying process, which includes fear of a slow death and a painful death". |
- | The [[Yale]] philosopher [[Shelly Kagan]] examined fear of death in a 2007 Yale open course | + | The [[Yale]] philosopher [[Shelly Kagan]] examined fear of death in a 2007 Yale open course by examining the following questions: Is fear of death a reasonable appropriate response? What conditions are required and what are appropriate conditions for feeling fear of death? What is meant by fear, and how much fear is appropriate? According to Kagan for fear in general to make sense, three conditions should be met: the object of fear needs to be "something bad", there needs to be a non-negligible chance of the bad state of affairs to happen, and there needs to be some uncertainty about the bad state of affairs. The amount of fear should be appropriate to the size of "the bad". If the 3 conditions aren't met, fear is an inappropriate emotion. He argues, that death does not meet the first two criteria, even if death is a "deprivation of good things" and even if one believes in a painful afterlife. Because death is certain, it also does not meet the third criteria, but he grants that the unpredictability of when one dies ''may'' be cause to a sense of fear. |
- | In a 2003 study of 167 women and 121 men, aged 65–87, low [[self-efficacy]] predicted fear of the unknown after death and fear of dying for women and men better than demographics, social support, and physical health. Fear of death was measured by a "Multidimensional Fear of Death Scale" which included the 8 subscales Fear of Dying, Fear of the Dead, Fear of Being Destroyed, Fear of Significant Others, Fear of the Unknown, Fear of Conscious Death, Fear for the Body After Death, and Fear of Premature Death. In [[hierarchical multiple regression]] analysis the most potent predictors of death fears were low "spiritual health efficacy", defined as beliefs relating to one's perceived ability to generate spiritually based faith and inner strength, and low "instrumental efficacy", defined as beliefs relating to one's perceived ability to manage activities of daily living. | + | In a 2003 study of 167 women and 121 men, aged 65–87, low [[self-efficacy]] predicted fear of the unknown after death and fear of dying for women and men better than demographics, social support, and physical health. Fear of death was measured by a "Multidimensional Fear of Death Scale" which included the 8 subscales Fear of Dying, Fear of the Dead, Fear of Being Destroyed, Fear of Significant Others, Fear of the Unknown, Fear of Conscious Death, Fear for the Body After Death, and Fear of Premature Death. In [[hierarchical multiple regression]] analysis the most potent predictors of death fears were low "spiritual health efficacy", defined as beliefs relating to one's perceived ability to generate spiritually based faith and inner strength, and low "instrumental efficacy", defined as beliefs relating to one's perceived ability to manage activities of daily living. |
- | Psychologists have tested the hypothesis that fear of death motivates religious commitment, and assurances about an afterlife alleviate the fear and empirical research on this topic has been equivocal.{{Citation needed|date=February 2010}} Religiosity can be related to fear of death when the afterlife is portrayed as time of punishment. "Intrinsic religiosity", as opposed to mere "formal religious involvement" has been found to be negatively correlated with death anxiety (reviewed in. | + | Psychologists have tested the hypothesis that fear of death motivates religious commitment, and assurances about an afterlife alleviate the fear and empirical research on this topic has been equivocal.{{Citation needed|date=February 2010}} Religiosity can be related to fear of death when the afterlife is portrayed as time of punishment. "Intrinsic religiosity", as opposed to mere "formal religious involvement" has been found to be negatively correlated with death anxiety (reviewed in. |
- | In a 1976 study people of various Christian denominations those most firm in their faith, attending religious services weekly were the least afraid of dying. The survey found a negative correlation between fear of death and "religious concern". | + | In a 1976 study people of various Christian denominations those most firm in their faith, attending religious services weekly were the least afraid of dying. The survey found a negative correlation between fear of death and "religious concern".{{Better source|reason=citation is not peer-reviewed|date=February 2014}} |
- | In a 2006 study of white, Christian men and women the hypothesis was tested that traditional, church-centered religiousness and de-institutionalized spiritual seeking are ways of approaching fear of death in old age. Both religiousness and spirituality were related to positive psychosocial functioning, but only church-centered religiousness protected subjects against the fear of death. | + | In a 2006 study of white, Christian men and women the hypothesis was tested that traditional, church-centered religiousness and de-institutionalized spiritual seeking are ways of approaching fear of death in old age. Both religiousness and spirituality were related to positive psychosocial functioning, but only church-centered religiousness protected subjects against the fear of death.{{Better source|reason=citation is not peer-reviewed|date=February 2014}} |
===Fear of the unknown=== | ===Fear of the unknown=== | ||
- | Many people are scared of the "unknown."{{citation needed|date=February 2014}} The unknown can branch out to many areas such as the hereafter, the next ten years, or even tomorrow. Many people are too scared to take the path they want to, because of what may lie ahead. Fear of the unknown is one of the reasons that people do not make an effort to enhance their scholarly education. However, if they do, most people would rather teach things they've been taught than go and do research on something new. They perceive this as a risk that may cause them fear and stress. | + | Many people are scared of the "unknown."{{citation needed|date=February 2014}} The unknown can branch out to many areas such as the hereafter, the next ten years, or even tomorrow. Many people are too scared to take the path they want to, because of what may lie ahead. Fear of the unknown is one of the reasons that people do not make an effort to enhance their scholarly education. However, if they do, most people would rather teach things they've been taught than go and do research on something new. They perceive this as a risk that may cause them fear and stress. This can lead to habits such as procrastination.{{Better source|reason=citation is not peer-reviewed|date=February 2014}} |
=== Fear of uncertainty and unpredictability === | === Fear of uncertainty and unpredictability === | ||
- | The stress of living in a constantly unpredictable environment can cause anxiety, other psychological problems and physical problems. People can develop fear to uncertainty. Parents tell their children not to talk to strangers in order to protect them. However, some research suggests we should not fear strangers, but be mindful of the risks that they could pose on children. | + | The stress of living in a constantly unpredictable environment can cause anxiety, other psychological problems and physical problems. People can develop fear to uncertainty. Parents tell their children not to talk to strangers in order to protect them. However, some research suggests we should not fear strangers, but be mindful of the risks that they could pose on children.{{Better source|reason=peer-reviewed opinion piece without citations|date=February 2014}} |
==Causes== | ==Causes== | ||
People develop specific fears as a result of learning. This has been studied in psychology as [[fear conditioning]], beginning with John B. Watson's [[Little Albert experiment]] in 1920, which was inspired after observing a child with an irrational fear of dogs. In this study, an 11-month-old boy was conditioned to fear a white rat in the laboratory. The fear became generalized to include other white, furry objects, such as a rabbit, dog, and even a ball of cotton. | People develop specific fears as a result of learning. This has been studied in psychology as [[fear conditioning]], beginning with John B. Watson's [[Little Albert experiment]] in 1920, which was inspired after observing a child with an irrational fear of dogs. In this study, an 11-month-old boy was conditioned to fear a white rat in the laboratory. The fear became generalized to include other white, furry objects, such as a rabbit, dog, and even a ball of cotton. | ||
- | Fear can be learned by experiencing or watching a frightening traumatic accident. For example, if a child falls into a well and struggles to get out, he or she may develop a fear of wells, heights ([[acrophobia]]), enclosed spaces ([[claustrophobia]]), or water ([[aquaphobia]]). There are studies looking at areas of the brain that are affected in relation to fear. When looking at these areas (such as the amygdala), it was proposed that a person learns to fear regardless of whether they themselves have experienced trauma, or if they have observed the fear in others. In a study completed by Andreas Olsson, Katherine I. Nearing and Elizabeth A. Phelps the amygdala were affected both when subjects observed someone else being submitted to an aversive event, knowing that the same treatment awaited themselves, and when subjects were subsequently placed in a fear-provoking situation. | + | Fear can be learned by experiencing or watching a frightening traumatic accident. For example, if a child falls into a well and struggles to get out, he or she may develop a fear of wells, heights ([[acrophobia]]), enclosed spaces ([[claustrophobia]]), or water ([[aquaphobia]]). There are studies looking at areas of the brain that are affected in relation to fear. When looking at these areas (such as the amygdala), it was proposed that a person learns to fear regardless of whether they themselves have experienced trauma, or if they have observed the fear in others. In a study completed by Andreas Olsson, Katherine I. Nearing and Elizabeth A. Phelps the amygdala were affected both when subjects observed someone else being submitted to an aversive event, knowing that the same treatment awaited themselves, and when subjects were subsequently placed in a fear-provoking situation. This suggests that fear can develop in both conditions, not just simply from personal history. |
Fear is affected by cultural and historical context. For example, in the early 20th century, many Americans feared [[polio]], a disease that cripples the body part it affects, leaving that body part immobilized for the rest of one's life.{{citation needed|date=February 2014}} There are consistent cross-cultural differences in how people respond to fear.{{citation needed|date=February 2014}} [[Display rules]] affect how likely people are to show the facial expression of fear and other emotions. | Fear is affected by cultural and historical context. For example, in the early 20th century, many Americans feared [[polio]], a disease that cripples the body part it affects, leaving that body part immobilized for the rest of one's life.{{citation needed|date=February 2014}} There are consistent cross-cultural differences in how people respond to fear.{{citation needed|date=February 2014}} [[Display rules]] affect how likely people are to show the facial expression of fear and other emotions. | ||
Line 70: | Line 70: | ||
Although many fears are learned, the capacity to fear is part of [[human nature]]. Many studies {{citation needed|date=February 2014}} have found that certain fears (e.g. animals, heights) are much more common than others (e.g. flowers, clouds). These fears are also easier to induce in the laboratory. This phenomenon is known as [[preparedness (learning)|preparedness]]. Because early humans that were quick to fear dangerous situations were more likely to survive and reproduce, preparedness is theorized to be a genetic effect that is the result of [[natural selection]] {{citation needed|date=February 2014}}. | Although many fears are learned, the capacity to fear is part of [[human nature]]. Many studies {{citation needed|date=February 2014}} have found that certain fears (e.g. animals, heights) are much more common than others (e.g. flowers, clouds). These fears are also easier to induce in the laboratory. This phenomenon is known as [[preparedness (learning)|preparedness]]. Because early humans that were quick to fear dangerous situations were more likely to survive and reproduce, preparedness is theorized to be a genetic effect that is the result of [[natural selection]] {{citation needed|date=February 2014}}. | ||
- | From an [[evolutionary psychology]] perspective, different fears may be different [[adaptation]]s that have been useful in our evolutionary past. They may have developed during different time periods. Some fears, such as fear of heights, may be common to all [[mammal]]s and developed during the [[mesozoic]] period. Other fears, such as fear of snakes, may be common to all [[simian]]s and developed during the [[cenozoic]] time period. Still others, such as fear of mice and insects, may be unique to humans and developed during the [[paleolithic]] and [[neolithic]] time periods (when mice and insects become important carriers of infectious diseases and harmful for crops and stored foods). | + | From an [[evolutionary psychology]] perspective, different fears may be different [[adaptation]]s that have been useful in our evolutionary past. They may have developed during different time periods. Some fears, such as fear of heights, may be common to all [[mammal]]s and developed during the [[mesozoic]] period. Other fears, such as fear of snakes, may be common to all [[simian]]s and developed during the [[cenozoic]] time period. Still others, such as fear of mice and insects, may be unique to humans and developed during the [[paleolithic]] and [[neolithic]] time periods (when mice and insects become important carriers of infectious diseases and harmful for crops and stored foods). |
- | Fear is high only if the observed risk and seriousness both are high, and is low, if risk or seriousness is low. | + | Fear is high only if the observed risk and seriousness both are high, and is low, if risk or seriousness is low. |
==Symptoms and signs of fear== | ==Symptoms and signs of fear== | ||
- | Many physiological changes in the body are associated with fear, summarized as the [[fight-or-flight response]]. An inborn response for coping with danger, it works by accelerating the breathing rate ([[hyperventilation]]), heart rate, constriction of the peripheral blood vessels leading to blushing and vasodilation of the central vessels (pooling), increasing muscle tension including the muscles attached to each hair follicle to contract and causing "goose bumps", or more clinically, [[piloerection]] (making a cold person warmer or a frightened animal look more impressive), sweating, increased blood glucose ([[hyperglycemia]]), increased serum calcium, increase in white blood cells called neutrophilic leukocytes, alertness leading to sleep disturbance and "butterflies in the stomach" ([[dyspepsia]]). This primitive mechanism may help an organism survive by either running away or fighting the danger. | + | Many physiological changes in the body are associated with fear, summarized as the [[fight-or-flight response]]. An inborn response for coping with danger, it works by accelerating the breathing rate ([[hyperventilation]]), heart rate, constriction of the peripheral blood vessels leading to blushing and vasodilation of the central vessels (pooling), increasing muscle tension including the muscles attached to each hair follicle to contract and causing "goose bumps", or more clinically, [[piloerection]] (making a cold person warmer or a frightened animal look more impressive), sweating, increased blood glucose ([[hyperglycemia]]), increased serum calcium, increase in white blood cells called neutrophilic leukocytes, alertness leading to sleep disturbance and "butterflies in the stomach" ([[dyspepsia]]). This primitive mechanism may help an organism survive by either running away or fighting the danger. With the series of physiological changes, the consciousness realizes an emotion of fear. |
== Fear in animals == | == Fear in animals == | ||
- | Often laboratory studies with rats are conducted to examine the acquisition and extinction of [[fear conditioning|conditioned fear]] responses. | + | Often laboratory studies with rats are conducted to examine the acquisition and extinction of [[fear conditioning|conditioned fear]] responses. The researchers were able to then cause an extinction of this conditioned fear, to a point that no medications or drugs were able to further aid in the extinction process. However the rats did show signs of avoidance learning, not fear, but simply avoiding the area that brought pain to the tests rats. The avoidance learning of rats is seen as a [[conditioned response]], and therefore the behavior can be unconditioned, as supported by the earlier research. |
- | Species-specific defense reactions (SSDRs) or [[avoidance learning]] in nature is the specific tendency to avoid certain threats or stimuli, it is how animals survive in the wild. Humans and animals both share these species-specific defense reactions, such as the flight, fight, which also include pseudo-aggression, fake or intimidating aggression, freeze response to threats, which is controlled by the [[sympathetic nervous system]]. These SSDRs are learned very quickly through social interactions between others of the same species, other species, and interaction with the environment. | + | Species-specific defense reactions (SSDRs) or [[avoidance learning]] in nature is the specific tendency to avoid certain threats or stimuli, it is how animals survive in the wild. Humans and animals both share these species-specific defense reactions, such as the flight, fight, which also include pseudo-aggression, fake or intimidating aggression, freeze response to threats, which is controlled by the [[sympathetic nervous system]]. These SSDRs are learned very quickly through social interactions between others of the same species, other species, and interaction with the environment. These acquired sets of reactions or responses are not easily forgotten. The animal that survives is the animal that already knows what to fear and how to avoid this threat. An example in humans is the reaction to the sight of a snake, many jump backwards before cognitively realizing what they are jumping away from, and in some cases it is a stick rather than a snake. |
- | As with many functions of the brain, there are various regions of the brain involved in deciphering fear in humans and other nonhuman species. | + | As with many functions of the brain, there are various regions of the brain involved in deciphering fear in humans and other nonhuman species. This impairment can cause different species to lack the sensation of fear, and often can become overly confident, confronting larger peers, or walking up to predatory creatures. |
- | [[Robert C. Bolles]] (1970), a researcher at University of Washington, wanted to understand species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning among animals, but found that the theories of avoidance learning and the tools that were used to measure this tendency were out of touch with the natural world. | + | [[Robert C. Bolles]] (1970), a researcher at University of Washington, wanted to understand species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning among animals, but found that the theories of avoidance learning and the tools that were used to measure this tendency were out of touch with the natural world. |
- | Species specific defense responses are created out of fear, and are essential for survival. | + | Species specific defense responses are created out of fear, and are essential for survival. Animals use these SSDR to continue living, to help increase their chance of [[Fitness (biology)|fitness]], by surviving long enough to procreate. Humans and animals alike have created fear to know what should avoided, and this fear can be learned through [[Learning|association]] with others in the community, or learned through personal experience with a creature, species, or situations that should be avoided. SSDRs are an evolutionary adaptation that has been seen in many species throughout the world including rats, [[chimpanzees]], [[prairie dogs]], and even [[humans]], an adaptation created to help individual creatures survive in a hostile world. |
==Neurocircuit of fear in mammals== | ==Neurocircuit of fear in mammals== | ||
Line 93: | Line 93: | ||
* Sensory cortex organizes information for dissemination to hypothalamus (fight or flight), amygdala (fear), hippocampus (memory) | * Sensory cortex organizes information for dissemination to hypothalamus (fight or flight), amygdala (fear), hippocampus (memory) | ||
- | The brain structure that is the center of most neurobiological events associated with fear is the [[amygdala]], located behind the pituitary gland. The amygdala is part of a circuitry of fear learning. | + | The brain structure that is the center of most neurobiological events associated with fear is the [[amygdala]], located behind the pituitary gland. The amygdala is part of a circuitry of fear learning. |
- | Some of the hormones involved during the state of fight-or-flight include epinephrine, norepinephrine and cortisol. Epinephrine regulates heart rate and metabolism as well as dilating blood vessels and air passages. [[Norepinephrine]] increases heart rate, blood flow to skeletal muscles and the release of glucose from energy stores. | + | Some of the hormones involved during the state of fight-or-flight include epinephrine, norepinephrine and cortisol. Epinephrine regulates heart rate and metabolism as well as dilating blood vessels and air passages. [[Norepinephrine]] increases heart rate, blood flow to skeletal muscles and the release of glucose from energy stores. |
- | After a situation which incites fear occurs, the amygdala and [[hippocampus]] record the event through synaptic [[neuroplasticity|plasticity]]. | + | After a situation which incites fear occurs, the amygdala and [[hippocampus]] record the event through synaptic [[neuroplasticity|plasticity]]. |
- | Pathogens can suppress amygdala activity. Rats infected with the [[toxoplasmosis]] parasite become less fearful of cats, sometimes even seeking out their urine-marked areas. This behavior often leads to them being eaten by cats. The parasite then reproduces within the body of the cat. There is evidence that the parasite concentrates itself in the amygdala of infected rats. | + | Pathogens can suppress amygdala activity. Rats infected with the [[toxoplasmosis]] parasite become less fearful of cats, sometimes even seeking out their urine-marked areas. This behavior often leads to them being eaten by cats. The parasite then reproduces within the body of the cat. There is evidence that the parasite concentrates itself in the amygdala of infected rats. |
- | Several brain structures other than the amygdala have also been observed to be activated when individuals are presented with fearful vs. neutral faces, namely the occipito[[cerebellum|cerebellar]] regions including the [[fusiform gyrus]] and the [[Inferior parietal lobule|inferior parietal]] / [[Superior temporal gyrus|superior temporal]] gyri. | + | Several brain structures other than the amygdala have also been observed to be activated when individuals are presented with fearful vs. neutral faces, namely the occipito[[cerebellum|cerebellar]] regions including the [[fusiform gyrus]] and the [[Inferior parietal lobule|inferior parietal]] / [[Superior temporal gyrus|superior temporal]] gyri. |
===Fear [[pheromones]] and why fear can be contagious=== | ===Fear [[pheromones]] and why fear can be contagious=== | ||
In threatening situations insects, aquatic organisms, birds, reptiles, and mammals emit odorant substances, initially called alarm substances, which are chemical signals now called alarm [[pheromones]] ("Schreckstoff" in German). This is to defend themselves and at the same time to inform members of the same species of danger and leads to observable behavior change like freezing, defensive behavior, or dispersion depending on circumstances and species. For example, stressed rats release odorant cues that cause other rats to move away from the source of the signal. Pheromones are synthesized, emitted and perceived by all living organisms studied to date, with the exception of viruses and prions: i.e. in bacteria, prokaryotes, plants, plankton, parasites, insects, invertebrates and vertebrates (aquatic organisms, birds, reptiles, and mammals). | In threatening situations insects, aquatic organisms, birds, reptiles, and mammals emit odorant substances, initially called alarm substances, which are chemical signals now called alarm [[pheromones]] ("Schreckstoff" in German). This is to defend themselves and at the same time to inform members of the same species of danger and leads to observable behavior change like freezing, defensive behavior, or dispersion depending on circumstances and species. For example, stressed rats release odorant cues that cause other rats to move away from the source of the signal. Pheromones are synthesized, emitted and perceived by all living organisms studied to date, with the exception of viruses and prions: i.e. in bacteria, prokaryotes, plants, plankton, parasites, insects, invertebrates and vertebrates (aquatic organisms, birds, reptiles, and mammals). | ||
- | After the discovery of pheromones in 1959, alarm pheromones were first described in 1968 in ants | + | After the discovery of pheromones in 1959, alarm pheromones were first described in 1968 in ants Over the next two decades identification and characterization of these pheromones proceeded in all manner of insects and sea animals, including fish, but it was not until 1990, that more insight into mammalian alarm pheromones was gleaned. |
- | Early on, in 1985 a link between odors released by stressed rats and [[pain perception]] was discovered in that unstressed rats developed opioid mediated analgesia | + | Early on, in 1985 a link between odors released by stressed rats and [[pain perception]] was discovered in that unstressed rats developed opioid mediated analgesia The experiment also showed that the bee’s fear-induced [[pain tolerance]] was mediated by an [[endorphine]]. |
- | By using the [[Behavioural despair test|forced swimming test]] in rats as a model of fear-induction, the first mammalian "alarm substance" was found. | + | By using the [[Behavioural despair test|forced swimming test]] in rats as a model of fear-induction, the first mammalian "alarm substance" was found. |
- | In 1991, this "alarm substance" was shown to fulfill criteria for pheromones: well-defined behavioral effect, species specificity, minimal influence of experience and control for nonspecific arousal. Rat activity testing with alarm pheromone and their preference/avoidance for odors from cylinders containing the pheromone showed, that the pheromone had very low [[Volatility (chemistry)|volatility]]. | + | In 1991, this "alarm substance" was shown to fulfill criteria for pheromones: well-defined behavioral effect, species specificity, minimal influence of experience and control for nonspecific arousal. Rat activity testing with alarm pheromone and their preference/avoidance for odors from cylinders containing the pheromone showed, that the pheromone had very low [[Volatility (chemistry)|volatility]]. |
- | In 1993 a connection between alarm chemosignals in mice and their [[immune system|immune response]] was found. | + | In 1993 a connection between alarm chemosignals in mice and their [[immune system|immune response]] was found. |
- | Pheromone production in mice was found to be associated with or mediated by the [[pituitary gland]] in 1994. | + | Pheromone production in mice was found to be associated with or mediated by the [[pituitary gland]] in 1994. |
- | It was not until 2011 that a link between severe pain, neuroinflammation and alarm pheromones release in rats was found: real time [[RT-PCR]] analysis of rat brain tissues indicated that shocking the footpad of a rat increased its production of [[proinflammatory cytokines]] in deep brain structures, namely of [[IL-1β]], heteronuclear [[Corticotropin-releasing hormone]] and [[c-fos]] mRNA expressions in both the [[paraventricular nucleus]] and the bed nucleus of the [[stria terminalis]], and it increased stress hormone levels in plasma ([[corticosterone]]). | + | It was not until 2011 that a link between severe pain, neuroinflammation and alarm pheromones release in rats was found: real time [[RT-PCR]] analysis of rat brain tissues indicated that shocking the footpad of a rat increased its production of [[proinflammatory cytokines]] in deep brain structures, namely of [[IL-1β]], heteronuclear [[Corticotropin-releasing hormone]] and [[c-fos]] mRNA expressions in both the [[paraventricular nucleus]] and the bed nucleus of the [[stria terminalis]], and it increased stress hormone levels in plasma ([[corticosterone]]). |
- | In 2004, it was demonstrated that rats’ alarm pheromones had different effects on the “recipient“ rat (the rat perceiving the pheromone) depending which body region they were released from: Pheromone production from the face modified behavior in the recipient rat, e.g. caused sniffing or movement, whereas pheromone secreted from the rat's anal area induced [[autonomic nervous system]] stress responses, like an increase in core body temperature. | + | In 2004, it was demonstrated that rats’ alarm pheromones had different effects on the “recipient“ rat (the rat perceiving the pheromone) depending which body region they were released from: Pheromone production from the face modified behavior in the recipient rat, e.g. caused sniffing or movement, whereas pheromone secreted from the rat's anal area induced [[autonomic nervous system]] stress responses, like an increase in core body temperature. and its acoustic [[startle reflex]] was enhanced. |
- | The [[emotion#neurocircuitry|neurocircuit]] for how rats perceive alarm pheromones was shown to be related to [[hypothalamus]], [[brainstem]], and [[amygdala]], all of which are evolutionary ancient structures deep inside or in the case of the brainstem underneath the brain away from the cortex, and involved in the [[Fight-or-flight response]], as is the case in humans. | + | The [[emotion#neurocircuitry|neurocircuit]] for how rats perceive alarm pheromones was shown to be related to [[hypothalamus]], [[brainstem]], and [[amygdala]], all of which are evolutionary ancient structures deep inside or in the case of the brainstem underneath the brain away from the cortex, and involved in the [[Fight-or-flight response]], as is the case in humans. |
- | Alarm pheromone-induced anxiety in rats has been used to evaluate the degree to which anxiolytics can alleviate anxiety in humans. For this the change in the [[startle response#acoustic startle reflex|acoutic startle reflex]] of rats with alarm pheromone-induced anxiety (i.e. reduction of defensiveness) has been measured. Pretreatment of rats with one of five [[anxiolytic]]s used in clinical medicine was able to reduce their anxiety: namely [[midazolam]], [[phenelzine]] (a nonselective monoamine oxidase (MAO) inhibitor), [[propranolol]], a nonselective [[beta blocker]], [[clonidine]], an [[alpha-2 adrenergic receptor#agonist|alpha 2 adrenergic agonist]] or [[CP-154,526]], a [[corticotropin-releasing hormone antagonist]]. | + | Alarm pheromone-induced anxiety in rats has been used to evaluate the degree to which anxiolytics can alleviate anxiety in humans. For this the change in the [[startle response#acoustic startle reflex|acoutic startle reflex]] of rats with alarm pheromone-induced anxiety (i.e. reduction of defensiveness) has been measured. Pretreatment of rats with one of five [[anxiolytic]]s used in clinical medicine was able to reduce their anxiety: namely [[midazolam]], [[phenelzine]] (a nonselective monoamine oxidase (MAO) inhibitor), [[propranolol]], a nonselective [[beta blocker]], [[clonidine]], an [[alpha-2 adrenergic receptor#agonist|alpha 2 adrenergic agonist]] or [[CP-154,526]], a [[corticotropin-releasing hormone antagonist]]. |
- | Faulty development of odor discrimination impairs the [[perception]] of pheromones and pheromone-related behavior, like [[aggression|aggressive behavior]] and mating in male rats: The enzyme [[MAPK7|Mitogen-activated protein kinase 7]] (MAPK7) has been implicated in regulating the development of the olfactory bulb and odor discrimination and it is highly expressed in developing rat brains, but absent in most regions of adult rat brains. [[Conditional gene knockout|conditional deletion]] of the MAPK7gene in mouse neural stem cells impairs several pheromone-mediated behaviors, including aggression and mating in male mice. These behavior impairments were not caused by a reduction in the level of testosterone, by physical immobility, by heightened fear or anxiety or by depression. Using mouse urine as a natural pheromone-containing solution, it has been shown that the impairment was associated with defective detection of related pheromones, and with changes in their inborn preference for pheromones related to sexual and reproductive activities. | + | Faulty development of odor discrimination impairs the [[perception]] of pheromones and pheromone-related behavior, like [[aggression|aggressive behavior]] and mating in male rats: The enzyme [[MAPK7|Mitogen-activated protein kinase 7]] (MAPK7) has been implicated in regulating the development of the olfactory bulb and odor discrimination and it is highly expressed in developing rat brains, but absent in most regions of adult rat brains. [[Conditional gene knockout|conditional deletion]] of the MAPK7gene in mouse neural stem cells impairs several pheromone-mediated behaviors, including aggression and mating in male mice. These behavior impairments were not caused by a reduction in the level of testosterone, by physical immobility, by heightened fear or anxiety or by depression. Using mouse urine as a natural pheromone-containing solution, it has been shown that the impairment was associated with defective detection of related pheromones, and with changes in their inborn preference for pheromones related to sexual and reproductive activities. |
- | Lastly, alleviation of an acute fear response because a friendly peer (or in biological language: an affiliative [[conspecific]]) [[tend and befriend|tends and befriends]] is called "social buffering". The term is in analogy to the 1985 "buffering" hypothesis in psychology, where [[social support]] has been proven to mitigate the negative health effects of alarm pheromone mediated distress. | + | Lastly, alleviation of an acute fear response because a friendly peer (or in biological language: an affiliative [[conspecific]]) [[tend and befriend|tends and befriends]] is called "social buffering". The term is in analogy to the 1985 "buffering" hypothesis in psychology, where [[social support]] has been proven to mitigate the negative health effects of alarm pheromone mediated distress. |
- | Biologists have proposed in 2012 that fear pheromones evolved as molecules of "keystone significance", a term coined in analogy to [[keystone species]]. Pheromones may determine [[species richness|species compositions]], and affect rates of energy and material exchange in an [[community(ecology)|ecological community]]. Thus pheromones generate structure in a [[food web|trophic web]] and play critical roles in maintaining natural systems. | + | Biologists have proposed in 2012 that fear pheromones evolved as molecules of "keystone significance", a term coined in analogy to [[keystone species]]. Pheromones may determine [[species richness|species compositions]], and affect rates of energy and material exchange in an [[community(ecology)|ecological community]]. Thus pheromones generate structure in a [[food web|trophic web]] and play critical roles in maintaining natural systems. |
===Fear pheromones in humans=== | ===Fear pheromones in humans=== | ||
- | Evidence of chemosensory alarm signals in humans has emerged slowly: Although alarm pheromones have not been physically isolated and their chemical structure has not been identified in man so far, there is evidence for their presence. [[Androstadienone]], for example, a steroidal, endogenous odorant, is a pheromone candidate found in human sweat, axillary hair and plasma. The closely related compound [[androstenone]] is involved in communicating dominance, aggression or competition; sex hormone influences on androstenone perception in humans showed high testosterone level related to heightened androstenone sensitivity in men, a high testosterone level related to [[unhappiness]] in response to androstenone in men, and a high estradiol level related to disliking of androstenone in women. | + | Evidence of chemosensory alarm signals in humans has emerged slowly: Although alarm pheromones have not been physically isolated and their chemical structure has not been identified in man so far, there is evidence for their presence. [[Androstadienone]], for example, a steroidal, endogenous odorant, is a pheromone candidate found in human sweat, axillary hair and plasma. The closely related compound [[androstenone]] is involved in communicating dominance, aggression or competition; sex hormone influences on androstenone perception in humans showed high testosterone level related to heightened androstenone sensitivity in men, a high testosterone level related to [[unhappiness]] in response to androstenone in men, and a high estradiol level related to disliking of androstenone in women. |
- | A German study from 2006 showed when anxiety-induced versus exercise-induced human sweat from a dozen people was pooled and offered to seven study participants, of five able to olfactorily distinguish exercise-induced sweat from room air, three could also distinguish exercise-induced sweat from anxiety induced sweat. The [[startle reflex|acoustic startle reflex]] response to a sound when sensing anxiety sweat was larger than when sensing exercise-induced sweat, as measured by [[electromyograph]] analysis of the orbital muscle, which is responsible for the eyeblink component. This showed for the first time that fear chemosignals can modulate the startle reflex in humans without emotional mediation; fear chemosignals primed the recipient's "defensive behavior" prior to the subjects' conscious attention on the acoustic startle reflex level. | + | A German study from 2006 showed when anxiety-induced versus exercise-induced human sweat from a dozen people was pooled and offered to seven study participants, of five able to olfactorily distinguish exercise-induced sweat from room air, three could also distinguish exercise-induced sweat from anxiety induced sweat. The [[startle reflex|acoustic startle reflex]] response to a sound when sensing anxiety sweat was larger than when sensing exercise-induced sweat, as measured by [[electromyograph]] analysis of the orbital muscle, which is responsible for the eyeblink component. This showed for the first time that fear chemosignals can modulate the startle reflex in humans without emotional mediation; fear chemosignals primed the recipient's "defensive behavior" prior to the subjects' conscious attention on the acoustic startle reflex level. |
- | In analogy to the social buffering of rats and honeybees in response to chemosignals, induction of empathy by smelling anxiety of another person has been found in humans. | + | In analogy to the social buffering of rats and honeybees in response to chemosignals, induction of empathy by smelling anxiety of another person has been found in humans. |
- | A study from 2013 provided brain imaging evidence that human responses to fear chemosignals may be gender-specific. Researchers collected alarm-induced sweat and exercise-induced sweat from donors extracted it, pooled it and presented it to 16 unrelated people undergoing functional brain [[MRI]]. While stress-induced sweat from males produced a comparably strong emotional response in both females and males, stress-induced sweat from females produced a markedly stronger arousal in women than in men. Statistical tests pinpointed this gender-specificity to the right amygdala and strongest in the superficial nuclei. Since no significant differences were found in the [[olfactory bulb]], the response to female fear-induced signals is likely based on processing the meaning, i.e. on the emotional level, rather than the strength of chemosensory cues from each gender, i.e. the perceptual level. | + | A study from 2013 provided brain imaging evidence that human responses to fear chemosignals may be gender-specific. Researchers collected alarm-induced sweat and exercise-induced sweat from donors extracted it, pooled it and presented it to 16 unrelated people undergoing functional brain [[MRI]]. While stress-induced sweat from males produced a comparably strong emotional response in both females and males, stress-induced sweat from females produced a markedly stronger arousal in women than in men. Statistical tests pinpointed this gender-specificity to the right amygdala and strongest in the superficial nuclei. Since no significant differences were found in the [[olfactory bulb]], the response to female fear-induced signals is likely based on processing the meaning, i.e. on the emotional level, rather than the strength of chemosensory cues from each gender, i.e. the perceptual level. |
- | An approach–avoidance task set up where volunteers seeing either an angry or a happy cartoon face on a computer screen pushed away or pulled toward them a joystick as fast as possible. Volunteers smelling anandrostadienone, masked with clove oil scent responded faster, especially to angry faces, than those smelling clove oil only, which was interpreted as anandrostadienone-related activation of the fear system. | + | An approach–avoidance task set up where volunteers seeing either an angry or a happy cartoon face on a computer screen pushed away or pulled toward them a joystick as fast as possible. Volunteers smelling anandrostadienone, masked with clove oil scent responded faster, especially to angry faces, than those smelling clove oil only, which was interpreted as anandrostadienone-related activation of the fear system. A potential mechanism of action is, that androstadienone alters the "emotional face processing". Androstadienone is known to influence activity of the [[fusiform gyrus]] which is relevant for [[face perception|face recognition]]. |
==Fears in culture== | ==Fears in culture== | ||
Line 155: | Line 155: | ||
=== Manipulation === | === Manipulation === | ||
- | Fear may be politically and culturally manipulated to dissuade citizenry about the implementation of market-oriented policies, otherwise would be widely rejected. In contexts of disasters, nation-states manage the fear not only to provide their citizens with an explanation about the event or blaming some minorities, but also to adjust their previous beliefs. The manipulation of fear is done by means of symbolic instruments as terror movies and the administration ideologies that lead to nationalism. After a disaster, the fear is re-channeled in a climate of euphoria based on patriotism. The fear and evilness are inextricably intertwined. | + | Fear may be politically and culturally manipulated to dissuade citizenry about the implementation of market-oriented policies, otherwise would be widely rejected. In contexts of disasters, nation-states manage the fear not only to provide their citizens with an explanation about the event or blaming some minorities, but also to adjust their previous beliefs. The manipulation of fear is done by means of symbolic instruments as terror movies and the administration ideologies that lead to nationalism. After a disaster, the fear is re-channeled in a climate of euphoria based on patriotism. The fear and evilness are inextricably intertwined. |
=== Mirroring fears === | === Mirroring fears === | ||
Line 164: | Line 164: | ||
===Pharmaceutical=== | ===Pharmaceutical=== | ||
- | A drug treatment for fear conditioning and phobias via the amygdala is the use of [[glucocorticoid]]s. | + | A drug treatment for fear conditioning and phobias via the amygdala is the use of [[glucocorticoid]]s. In one study, glucocorticoid receptors in the central nucleus of the amygdala were disrupted in order to better understand the mechanisms of fear and fear conditioning. The glucocorticoid receptors were inhibited using lentiviral vectors containing Cre-recombinase injected into mice. Results showed that disruption of the glucocorticoid receptors prevented conditioned fear behavior. The mice were subjected to auditory cues which caused them to freeze normally. However, a reduction of freezing was observed in the mice that had inhibited glucocorticoid receptors. |
===[[Psychology]]=== | ===[[Psychology]]=== | ||
- | Cognitive behavioral therapy has been successful in helping people overcome fear. Because fear is more complex than just forgetting or deleting memories, an active and successful approach involves people repeatedly confronting their fears. By confronting their fears—in a safe manner—a person can suppress the fear-triggering memory or stimulus. Known as ‘[[exposure therapy]]’, this practice can help cure up to 90% of people, with specific [[phobias]]. | + | Cognitive behavioral therapy has been successful in helping people overcome fear. Because fear is more complex than just forgetting or deleting memories, an active and successful approach involves people repeatedly confronting their fears. By confronting their fears—in a safe manner—a person can suppress the fear-triggering memory or stimulus. Known as ‘[[exposure therapy]]’, this practice can help cure up to 90% of people, with specific [[phobias]]. |
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
==External links== | ==External links== | ||
- | |||
- | |||
- | |||
* [http://www.nel.edu/23_2/NEL230202R01_Grammer.htm The Scent of Fear, a Research Study] | * [http://www.nel.edu/23_2/NEL230202R01_Grammer.htm The Scent of Fear, a Research Study] | ||
* [http://www.newadvent.org/cathen/06021a.htm ''Catholic Encyclopedia'' "Fear (from a Moral Standpoint)"] | * [http://www.newadvent.org/cathen/06021a.htm ''Catholic Encyclopedia'' "Fear (from a Moral Standpoint)"] |
Revision as of 12:00, 23 August 2014
About the Project
What is it?
Contents |
Fear is an emotion induced by a threat perceived by living entities, which causes a change in brain and organ function and ultimately a change in behavior, such as running away, hiding or freezing from traumatic events. Fear may occur in response to a specific stimulus happening in the present, or to a future situation, which is perceived as risk to health or life, status, power, security, or in the case of humans wealth or anything held valuable. The fear response arises from the perception of danger leading to confrontation with or escape from/avoiding the threat (also known as the fight-or-flight response), which in extreme cases of fear (horror and terror) can be a freeze response or paralysis.
In humans and animals, fear is modulated by the process of cognition and learning. Thus fear is judged as rational or appropriate and irrational or inappropriate. An irrational fear is called a phobia.
Psychologists such as John B. Watson, Robert Plutchik, and Paul Ekman have suggested that there is only a small set of basic or innate emotions and that fear is one of them. This hypothesized set includes such emotions as joy, sadness, fright, dread, horror, panic, anxiety, acute stress reaction and anger.
Fear should be distinguished from, but is closely related to, the emotion anxiety, which occurs as the result of threats which are perceived to be uncontrollable or unavoidable.
The fear response serves survival by generating appropriate behavioral responses, as it has been preserved throughout evolution.
Etymology
The noun fear stems from the Middle English feer, fere, fer, the Old English fǣr for 'calamity, danger' (and its verb fǣran 'frighten', but also 'revere') is related to the Proto-Germanic fērą 'danger', the Proto-Indo-European *per- 'to attempt, try, research, risk'. Fear is translated into German with Gefahr, into Dutch with gevaar, into Swedish with fara, into Albanian with frikë, and into Latin with perīculum, which is the root for the term in the Romanic languages.
The noun fear can be used in three ways with different meanings: In the uncountable form fear is a strong, uncontrollable, unpleasant emotion caused by actual or perceived danger, e.g. 'He was struck by fear on seeing the snake'. In the countable form, and when used with the indefinite article "a fear" means a phobia, a sense of fear induced by something or someone, e.g. 'Not everybody has the same fears; I have a fear of ants.' In an uncountable form it can also mean extreme veneration or awe, as toward a supreme being or deity.
Types
Top 10 types of fear in the U.S.
In a 2005 Gallup poll (U.S.A.), a national sample of adolescents between the ages of 13 and 17 were asked what they feared the most. The question was open-ended and participants were able to say whatever they wanted. The top ten fears were, in order: terrorist attacks, spiders, death, being a failure, war, criminal or gang violence, being alone, the future, and nuclear war.
In an estimate of what people fear the most, book author Bill Tancer analyzed the most frequent online queries that involved the phrase, "fear of..." following the assumption that people tend to seek information on the issues that concern them the most. His top ten list of fears published 2008 consisted of flying, heights, clowns, intimacy, death, rejection, people, snakes, failure, and driving.
Common phobias
Template:See also According to surveys Template:Citation needed, some of the most common fears are of demons and ghosts, the existence of evil powers, cockroaches, spiders, snakes, heights, water, enclosed spaces, tunnels, bridges, needles, social rejection, failure, examinations and public speaking.
One of the most common fears in humans is the fear of public speaking. People may be comfortable speaking inside a room but when it becomes public speaking, fear enters in the form of suspicion over whether the words uttered are correct or incorrect because there are many to judge them. Another common fear can be of pain, or of someone damaging a person. Fear of pain in a plausible situation brings flinching, or cringing.
Fear of survival
According to Irfan Jamil, coadjutor bishop of Lahore, as the world constantly changes, the greatest fear is the fear of survival. The social, economical, spiritual, political and educational circumstances in life make survival difficult in regard to such pressures that can come out of it. Template:Better source
Fear of death
Death anxiety is multidimensional; it covers "fears related to one's own death, the death of others, fear of the unknown after death, fear of obliteration, and fear of the dying process, which includes fear of a slow death and a painful death".
The Yale philosopher Shelly Kagan examined fear of death in a 2007 Yale open course by examining the following questions: Is fear of death a reasonable appropriate response? What conditions are required and what are appropriate conditions for feeling fear of death? What is meant by fear, and how much fear is appropriate? According to Kagan for fear in general to make sense, three conditions should be met: the object of fear needs to be "something bad", there needs to be a non-negligible chance of the bad state of affairs to happen, and there needs to be some uncertainty about the bad state of affairs. The amount of fear should be appropriate to the size of "the bad". If the 3 conditions aren't met, fear is an inappropriate emotion. He argues, that death does not meet the first two criteria, even if death is a "deprivation of good things" and even if one believes in a painful afterlife. Because death is certain, it also does not meet the third criteria, but he grants that the unpredictability of when one dies may be cause to a sense of fear.
In a 2003 study of 167 women and 121 men, aged 65–87, low self-efficacy predicted fear of the unknown after death and fear of dying for women and men better than demographics, social support, and physical health. Fear of death was measured by a "Multidimensional Fear of Death Scale" which included the 8 subscales Fear of Dying, Fear of the Dead, Fear of Being Destroyed, Fear of Significant Others, Fear of the Unknown, Fear of Conscious Death, Fear for the Body After Death, and Fear of Premature Death. In hierarchical multiple regression analysis the most potent predictors of death fears were low "spiritual health efficacy", defined as beliefs relating to one's perceived ability to generate spiritually based faith and inner strength, and low "instrumental efficacy", defined as beliefs relating to one's perceived ability to manage activities of daily living.
Psychologists have tested the hypothesis that fear of death motivates religious commitment, and assurances about an afterlife alleviate the fear and empirical research on this topic has been equivocal.Template:Citation needed Religiosity can be related to fear of death when the afterlife is portrayed as time of punishment. "Intrinsic religiosity", as opposed to mere "formal religious involvement" has been found to be negatively correlated with death anxiety (reviewed in.
In a 1976 study people of various Christian denominations those most firm in their faith, attending religious services weekly were the least afraid of dying. The survey found a negative correlation between fear of death and "religious concern".Template:Better source
In a 2006 study of white, Christian men and women the hypothesis was tested that traditional, church-centered religiousness and de-institutionalized spiritual seeking are ways of approaching fear of death in old age. Both religiousness and spirituality were related to positive psychosocial functioning, but only church-centered religiousness protected subjects against the fear of death.Template:Better source
Fear of the unknown
Many people are scared of the "unknown."Template:Citation needed The unknown can branch out to many areas such as the hereafter, the next ten years, or even tomorrow. Many people are too scared to take the path they want to, because of what may lie ahead. Fear of the unknown is one of the reasons that people do not make an effort to enhance their scholarly education. However, if they do, most people would rather teach things they've been taught than go and do research on something new. They perceive this as a risk that may cause them fear and stress. This can lead to habits such as procrastination.Template:Better source
Fear of uncertainty and unpredictability
The stress of living in a constantly unpredictable environment can cause anxiety, other psychological problems and physical problems. People can develop fear to uncertainty. Parents tell their children not to talk to strangers in order to protect them. However, some research suggests we should not fear strangers, but be mindful of the risks that they could pose on children.Template:Better source
Causes
People develop specific fears as a result of learning. This has been studied in psychology as fear conditioning, beginning with John B. Watson's Little Albert experiment in 1920, which was inspired after observing a child with an irrational fear of dogs. In this study, an 11-month-old boy was conditioned to fear a white rat in the laboratory. The fear became generalized to include other white, furry objects, such as a rabbit, dog, and even a ball of cotton.
Fear can be learned by experiencing or watching a frightening traumatic accident. For example, if a child falls into a well and struggles to get out, he or she may develop a fear of wells, heights (acrophobia), enclosed spaces (claustrophobia), or water (aquaphobia). There are studies looking at areas of the brain that are affected in relation to fear. When looking at these areas (such as the amygdala), it was proposed that a person learns to fear regardless of whether they themselves have experienced trauma, or if they have observed the fear in others. In a study completed by Andreas Olsson, Katherine I. Nearing and Elizabeth A. Phelps the amygdala were affected both when subjects observed someone else being submitted to an aversive event, knowing that the same treatment awaited themselves, and when subjects were subsequently placed in a fear-provoking situation. This suggests that fear can develop in both conditions, not just simply from personal history.
Fear is affected by cultural and historical context. For example, in the early 20th century, many Americans feared polio, a disease that cripples the body part it affects, leaving that body part immobilized for the rest of one's life.Template:Citation needed There are consistent cross-cultural differences in how people respond to fear.Template:Citation needed Display rules affect how likely people are to show the facial expression of fear and other emotions.
Although many fears are learned, the capacity to fear is part of human nature. Many studies Template:Citation needed have found that certain fears (e.g. animals, heights) are much more common than others (e.g. flowers, clouds). These fears are also easier to induce in the laboratory. This phenomenon is known as preparedness. Because early humans that were quick to fear dangerous situations were more likely to survive and reproduce, preparedness is theorized to be a genetic effect that is the result of natural selection Template:Citation needed.
From an evolutionary psychology perspective, different fears may be different adaptations that have been useful in our evolutionary past. They may have developed during different time periods. Some fears, such as fear of heights, may be common to all mammals and developed during the mesozoic period. Other fears, such as fear of snakes, may be common to all simians and developed during the cenozoic time period. Still others, such as fear of mice and insects, may be unique to humans and developed during the paleolithic and neolithic time periods (when mice and insects become important carriers of infectious diseases and harmful for crops and stored foods).
Fear is high only if the observed risk and seriousness both are high, and is low, if risk or seriousness is low.
Symptoms and signs of fear
Many physiological changes in the body are associated with fear, summarized as the fight-or-flight response. An inborn response for coping with danger, it works by accelerating the breathing rate (hyperventilation), heart rate, constriction of the peripheral blood vessels leading to blushing and vasodilation of the central vessels (pooling), increasing muscle tension including the muscles attached to each hair follicle to contract and causing "goose bumps", or more clinically, piloerection (making a cold person warmer or a frightened animal look more impressive), sweating, increased blood glucose (hyperglycemia), increased serum calcium, increase in white blood cells called neutrophilic leukocytes, alertness leading to sleep disturbance and "butterflies in the stomach" (dyspepsia). This primitive mechanism may help an organism survive by either running away or fighting the danger. With the series of physiological changes, the consciousness realizes an emotion of fear.
Fear in animals
Often laboratory studies with rats are conducted to examine the acquisition and extinction of conditioned fear responses. The researchers were able to then cause an extinction of this conditioned fear, to a point that no medications or drugs were able to further aid in the extinction process. However the rats did show signs of avoidance learning, not fear, but simply avoiding the area that brought pain to the tests rats. The avoidance learning of rats is seen as a conditioned response, and therefore the behavior can be unconditioned, as supported by the earlier research. Species-specific defense reactions (SSDRs) or avoidance learning in nature is the specific tendency to avoid certain threats or stimuli, it is how animals survive in the wild. Humans and animals both share these species-specific defense reactions, such as the flight, fight, which also include pseudo-aggression, fake or intimidating aggression, freeze response to threats, which is controlled by the sympathetic nervous system. These SSDRs are learned very quickly through social interactions between others of the same species, other species, and interaction with the environment. These acquired sets of reactions or responses are not easily forgotten. The animal that survives is the animal that already knows what to fear and how to avoid this threat. An example in humans is the reaction to the sight of a snake, many jump backwards before cognitively realizing what they are jumping away from, and in some cases it is a stick rather than a snake.
As with many functions of the brain, there are various regions of the brain involved in deciphering fear in humans and other nonhuman species. This impairment can cause different species to lack the sensation of fear, and often can become overly confident, confronting larger peers, or walking up to predatory creatures.
Robert C. Bolles (1970), a researcher at University of Washington, wanted to understand species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning among animals, but found that the theories of avoidance learning and the tools that were used to measure this tendency were out of touch with the natural world.
Species specific defense responses are created out of fear, and are essential for survival. Animals use these SSDR to continue living, to help increase their chance of fitness, by surviving long enough to procreate. Humans and animals alike have created fear to know what should avoided, and this fear can be learned through association with others in the community, or learned through personal experience with a creature, species, or situations that should be avoided. SSDRs are an evolutionary adaptation that has been seen in many species throughout the world including rats, chimpanzees, prairie dogs, and even humans, an adaptation created to help individual creatures survive in a hostile world.
Neurocircuit of fear in mammals
- The thalamus collects sensory data from the senses
- Sensory cortex receives data from thalamus and interprets it
- Sensory cortex organizes information for dissemination to hypothalamus (fight or flight), amygdala (fear), hippocampus (memory)
The brain structure that is the center of most neurobiological events associated with fear is the amygdala, located behind the pituitary gland. The amygdala is part of a circuitry of fear learning.
Some of the hormones involved during the state of fight-or-flight include epinephrine, norepinephrine and cortisol. Epinephrine regulates heart rate and metabolism as well as dilating blood vessels and air passages. Norepinephrine increases heart rate, blood flow to skeletal muscles and the release of glucose from energy stores.
After a situation which incites fear occurs, the amygdala and hippocampus record the event through synaptic plasticity.
Pathogens can suppress amygdala activity. Rats infected with the toxoplasmosis parasite become less fearful of cats, sometimes even seeking out their urine-marked areas. This behavior often leads to them being eaten by cats. The parasite then reproduces within the body of the cat. There is evidence that the parasite concentrates itself in the amygdala of infected rats.
Several brain structures other than the amygdala have also been observed to be activated when individuals are presented with fearful vs. neutral faces, namely the occipitocerebellar regions including the fusiform gyrus and the inferior parietal / superior temporal gyri.
Fear pheromones and why fear can be contagious
In threatening situations insects, aquatic organisms, birds, reptiles, and mammals emit odorant substances, initially called alarm substances, which are chemical signals now called alarm pheromones ("Schreckstoff" in German). This is to defend themselves and at the same time to inform members of the same species of danger and leads to observable behavior change like freezing, defensive behavior, or dispersion depending on circumstances and species. For example, stressed rats release odorant cues that cause other rats to move away from the source of the signal. Pheromones are synthesized, emitted and perceived by all living organisms studied to date, with the exception of viruses and prions: i.e. in bacteria, prokaryotes, plants, plankton, parasites, insects, invertebrates and vertebrates (aquatic organisms, birds, reptiles, and mammals).
After the discovery of pheromones in 1959, alarm pheromones were first described in 1968 in ants Over the next two decades identification and characterization of these pheromones proceeded in all manner of insects and sea animals, including fish, but it was not until 1990, that more insight into mammalian alarm pheromones was gleaned.
Early on, in 1985 a link between odors released by stressed rats and pain perception was discovered in that unstressed rats developed opioid mediated analgesia The experiment also showed that the bee’s fear-induced pain tolerance was mediated by an endorphine.
By using the forced swimming test in rats as a model of fear-induction, the first mammalian "alarm substance" was found.
In 1991, this "alarm substance" was shown to fulfill criteria for pheromones: well-defined behavioral effect, species specificity, minimal influence of experience and control for nonspecific arousal. Rat activity testing with alarm pheromone and their preference/avoidance for odors from cylinders containing the pheromone showed, that the pheromone had very low volatility.
In 1993 a connection between alarm chemosignals in mice and their immune response was found.
Pheromone production in mice was found to be associated with or mediated by the pituitary gland in 1994.
It was not until 2011 that a link between severe pain, neuroinflammation and alarm pheromones release in rats was found: real time RT-PCR analysis of rat brain tissues indicated that shocking the footpad of a rat increased its production of proinflammatory cytokines in deep brain structures, namely of IL-1β, heteronuclear Corticotropin-releasing hormone and c-fos mRNA expressions in both the paraventricular nucleus and the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, and it increased stress hormone levels in plasma (corticosterone).
In 2004, it was demonstrated that rats’ alarm pheromones had different effects on the “recipient“ rat (the rat perceiving the pheromone) depending which body region they were released from: Pheromone production from the face modified behavior in the recipient rat, e.g. caused sniffing or movement, whereas pheromone secreted from the rat's anal area induced autonomic nervous system stress responses, like an increase in core body temperature. and its acoustic startle reflex was enhanced.
The neurocircuit for how rats perceive alarm pheromones was shown to be related to hypothalamus, brainstem, and amygdala, all of which are evolutionary ancient structures deep inside or in the case of the brainstem underneath the brain away from the cortex, and involved in the Fight-or-flight response, as is the case in humans.
Alarm pheromone-induced anxiety in rats has been used to evaluate the degree to which anxiolytics can alleviate anxiety in humans. For this the change in the acoutic startle reflex of rats with alarm pheromone-induced anxiety (i.e. reduction of defensiveness) has been measured. Pretreatment of rats with one of five anxiolytics used in clinical medicine was able to reduce their anxiety: namely midazolam, phenelzine (a nonselective monoamine oxidase (MAO) inhibitor), propranolol, a nonselective beta blocker, clonidine, an alpha 2 adrenergic agonist or CP-154,526, a corticotropin-releasing hormone antagonist.
Faulty development of odor discrimination impairs the perception of pheromones and pheromone-related behavior, like aggressive behavior and mating in male rats: The enzyme Mitogen-activated protein kinase 7 (MAPK7) has been implicated in regulating the development of the olfactory bulb and odor discrimination and it is highly expressed in developing rat brains, but absent in most regions of adult rat brains. conditional deletion of the MAPK7gene in mouse neural stem cells impairs several pheromone-mediated behaviors, including aggression and mating in male mice. These behavior impairments were not caused by a reduction in the level of testosterone, by physical immobility, by heightened fear or anxiety or by depression. Using mouse urine as a natural pheromone-containing solution, it has been shown that the impairment was associated with defective detection of related pheromones, and with changes in their inborn preference for pheromones related to sexual and reproductive activities.
Lastly, alleviation of an acute fear response because a friendly peer (or in biological language: an affiliative conspecific) tends and befriends is called "social buffering". The term is in analogy to the 1985 "buffering" hypothesis in psychology, where social support has been proven to mitigate the negative health effects of alarm pheromone mediated distress.
Biologists have proposed in 2012 that fear pheromones evolved as molecules of "keystone significance", a term coined in analogy to keystone species. Pheromones may determine species compositions, and affect rates of energy and material exchange in an ecological community. Thus pheromones generate structure in a trophic web and play critical roles in maintaining natural systems.
Fear pheromones in humans
Evidence of chemosensory alarm signals in humans has emerged slowly: Although alarm pheromones have not been physically isolated and their chemical structure has not been identified in man so far, there is evidence for their presence. Androstadienone, for example, a steroidal, endogenous odorant, is a pheromone candidate found in human sweat, axillary hair and plasma. The closely related compound androstenone is involved in communicating dominance, aggression or competition; sex hormone influences on androstenone perception in humans showed high testosterone level related to heightened androstenone sensitivity in men, a high testosterone level related to unhappiness in response to androstenone in men, and a high estradiol level related to disliking of androstenone in women.
A German study from 2006 showed when anxiety-induced versus exercise-induced human sweat from a dozen people was pooled and offered to seven study participants, of five able to olfactorily distinguish exercise-induced sweat from room air, three could also distinguish exercise-induced sweat from anxiety induced sweat. The acoustic startle reflex response to a sound when sensing anxiety sweat was larger than when sensing exercise-induced sweat, as measured by electromyograph analysis of the orbital muscle, which is responsible for the eyeblink component. This showed for the first time that fear chemosignals can modulate the startle reflex in humans without emotional mediation; fear chemosignals primed the recipient's "defensive behavior" prior to the subjects' conscious attention on the acoustic startle reflex level.
In analogy to the social buffering of rats and honeybees in response to chemosignals, induction of empathy by smelling anxiety of another person has been found in humans.
A study from 2013 provided brain imaging evidence that human responses to fear chemosignals may be gender-specific. Researchers collected alarm-induced sweat and exercise-induced sweat from donors extracted it, pooled it and presented it to 16 unrelated people undergoing functional brain MRI. While stress-induced sweat from males produced a comparably strong emotional response in both females and males, stress-induced sweat from females produced a markedly stronger arousal in women than in men. Statistical tests pinpointed this gender-specificity to the right amygdala and strongest in the superficial nuclei. Since no significant differences were found in the olfactory bulb, the response to female fear-induced signals is likely based on processing the meaning, i.e. on the emotional level, rather than the strength of chemosensory cues from each gender, i.e. the perceptual level.
An approach–avoidance task set up where volunteers seeing either an angry or a happy cartoon face on a computer screen pushed away or pulled toward them a joystick as fast as possible. Volunteers smelling anandrostadienone, masked with clove oil scent responded faster, especially to angry faces, than those smelling clove oil only, which was interpreted as anandrostadienone-related activation of the fear system. A potential mechanism of action is, that androstadienone alters the "emotional face processing". Androstadienone is known to influence activity of the fusiform gyrus which is relevant for face recognition.
Fears in culture
Death
The fear of the end and its existence is in other words the fear of death. The fear of death ritualized the lives of our ancestors. These rituals were designed to reduce that fear; they helped collect the cultural ideas that we now have in the present. These rituals also helped preserve the cultural ideas. The results and methods of human existence had been changing at the same time that social formation was changing. One can say that the formation of communities happened because people lived in fear. The result of this fear forced people to unite to fight dangers together rather than fight alone.
Religion
Religions are filled with different fears that humans have had throughout many centuries. The fears aren't just metaphysical (including the problems of life and death) but are also moral. Death is seen as a boundary to another world. That world would always be different depending on how each individual lived their lives. The origins of this intangible fear are not found in the present world. In a sense we can assume that fear was a big influence on things such as morality.
There is another fear in the Bible that has a different meaning; the fear of God. Fear is used to express a Filial or a slavish passion. In believers the fear of god is "holy awe" or "reverence" of a particular god and the laws of its associated religion.
Manipulation
Fear may be politically and culturally manipulated to dissuade citizenry about the implementation of market-oriented policies, otherwise would be widely rejected. In contexts of disasters, nation-states manage the fear not only to provide their citizens with an explanation about the event or blaming some minorities, but also to adjust their previous beliefs. The manipulation of fear is done by means of symbolic instruments as terror movies and the administration ideologies that lead to nationalism. After a disaster, the fear is re-channeled in a climate of euphoria based on patriotism. The fear and evilness are inextricably intertwined.
Mirroring fears
Template:Unreferenced section Fear is found in mythology and folklore superstitions, and portrayed in books and movies. For example, many stories include characters who fear the antagonist of the plot. One of the important characteristics of historical and mythical heroes across cultures is to be fearless in the face of big and often lethal enemies.
Overcoming fear
Pharmaceutical
A drug treatment for fear conditioning and phobias via the amygdala is the use of glucocorticoids. In one study, glucocorticoid receptors in the central nucleus of the amygdala were disrupted in order to better understand the mechanisms of fear and fear conditioning. The glucocorticoid receptors were inhibited using lentiviral vectors containing Cre-recombinase injected into mice. Results showed that disruption of the glucocorticoid receptors prevented conditioned fear behavior. The mice were subjected to auditory cues which caused them to freeze normally. However, a reduction of freezing was observed in the mice that had inhibited glucocorticoid receptors.
Psychology
Cognitive behavioral therapy has been successful in helping people overcome fear. Because fear is more complex than just forgetting or deleting memories, an active and successful approach involves people repeatedly confronting their fears. By confronting their fears—in a safe manner—a person can suppress the fear-triggering memory or stimulus. Known as ‘exposure therapy’, this practice can help cure up to 90% of people, with specific phobias.
External links
- [http://www.nel.edu/23_2/NEL230202R01_Grammer.htm The Scent of Fear, a Research Study]
- [http://www.newadvent.org/cathen/06021a.htm Catholic Encyclopedia "Fear (from a Moral Standpoint)"]